Jokes and Follies
Baron von Stengel built an extravagant folly atop the Aurora
Cliff – a chimera that was part Moorish fantasy, part Scots castle, and part
optical illusion. In 1912 the castle that would be known as Swallow’s Nest
appeared on its dizzying perch 130 feet above the Black Sea. To build it Von
Stengel, rich from oil, had first demolished its predecessor, a timber cottage
known as the Castle of Love. Was it the oil magnate’s sly wit, or the wit of
his architect, Leonid Sherwood, that inspired the creation of one of the world’s best
visual jokes?
To reach Swallow’s Nest, you climb 800 steps down to the
base of the cliff, then steeply up. Glancing above you on a clear day, you see the castle set
against the bluest of blue skies: a castle with a two-story tower and
decorative crenellations, appearing now imposing, now inviting. As your magical
destination shimmers before you, you climb alongside other tourists, many of
them young Ukrainian couples with or without children. When you reach the top, as
I did on my daughter’s arm, out of breath, euphoric, you see the joke at once. The
castle viewed from a distance looms large; up close it is revealed to be a
small replica, only 66 feet long by 33 feet wide. The joke is on you – and you
will enjoy it one more time on the return ferry to Yalta, looking back up at the silhouette
against the sky.
Exegi monumentum, boasted
Horace: “I have raised a monument more permanent than bronze.” Exegi monumentum, wrote Pushkin, “I have
erected a monument to myself.” Poetry, especially the high art of Horace and
Pushkin, rightly lays claim to the permanency of a bronze statue. But Exegi monumentum, said the oil baron?
What claim to immortality did he make
with his fantasy castle, if not the claim of laughter? His is the claim of a
prankster, one who raised a monument to whimsy.
Rarely monumental, jokes are typically quick and transitory.
Jokes are far less often visual than verbal. Swallow’s Nest, surrounded by sea,
with sky as its backdrop, high up in the air, is perhaps the world’s most
atypical joke.
Freud, an expert on jokes as well as dreams, offered the
considered hypothesis that jokes are formed in the unconscious. Jokes surface
from the Netherworld of the mind after “a
preconscious thought is given over for a moment to unconscious revision and the
outcome of this is at once grasped by conscious perception.” (Freud,
Sigmund, Jokes and their Relation to the
Unconscious, 1905, p. 166) The place where revision occurs is an underworld
into which the half-formed joke-thought sinks. A joke is formed with “the
sinking of a preconscious thought into the unconscious and its unconscious
revision.” (p. 167)
Where then is the unconscious located, this part of the mind
into which the preconscious thought sinks? When the unconscious is pictured
metaphorically, its location is nearly always down below. Unconscious thoughts
exist below the surface, at a depth, undersea. The metaphor of unconscious
depths may be so prevalent because we think of the depths as a place of
transformation.
Full fathom five thy father
lies;
Of his bones are coral made;
Those are pearls that were his eyes:
Nothing of him that doth fade
But doth suffer a sea-change
Into something rich and strange.
Sea-nymphs hourly ring his knell:
Ding-dong.
Hark! now I hear them,--ding-dong, bell.
Of his bones are coral made;
Those are pearls that were his eyes:
Nothing of him that doth fade
But doth suffer a sea-change
Into something rich and strange.
Sea-nymphs hourly ring his knell:
Ding-dong.
Hark! now I hear them,--ding-dong, bell.
The
Tempest, Shakespeare, 1610
Ariel’s
song
Like
the fragments of a dream remembered, the thought that becomes a joke is
transformed in the unconscious. Before writing his study of jokes, Freud’s work
with patients had led to his discovery of how our dream thoughts
are shaped by the two tropes that rule dream language: condensation and
displacement. Freud deduced that a joke is formed – more quickly – but by
techniques similar to those operating in the unconscious production of dreams. Through
condensation (abbreviation, ellipsis, the part standing for the whole) or
displacement (allusion, exaggeration, understatement, the representation of the
opposite), the joke-thought metamorphoses into something rich and strange.
Transformation like this can only take place full fathom five.
We
may use the metaphor of unconscious depths because we believe that the unconscious
mind contains hidden knowledge. Filtered light barely reaches far enough into
the depths to illuminate our secret understanding. Our hidden knowledge is a
valuable possession, a deep-sea treasure that glows with its own strange light.
Full
many a gem of purest ray serene
The dark unfathom'd caves of ocean bear:
Full many a flower is born to blush unseen,
And waste its sweetness on the desert air.
The dark unfathom'd caves of ocean bear:
Full many a flower is born to blush unseen,
And waste its sweetness on the desert air.
“Elegy Written in a Country Churchyard,” Thomas
Gray, 1751
Clarification of the meaning of our dreams can be
gained by using the “mutilated and altered transcript” of our mental processes,
the dream fragments that surface in memory, to deduce the intelligible, secret
dream-thoughts hidden in the depths. (Freud, p. 160)
Feelings, like dream thoughts, like a joke taking
shape, can be buried deep in the unconscious mind. When Billie Holiday sings
about finding her lover “asleep/ In the deep of my heart”, we know that even as
the singer wakes from her dream, the image of her lover remains intact in the
unfathomed depths of her being.
Dreaming
I was only dreaming
I wake and I find you asleep
In the deep of my heart dear
Darling I hope
That my dream never haunted you
My heart is telling you
How much I wanted you
Gloomy Sunday
I was only dreaming
I wake and I find you asleep
In the deep of my heart dear
Darling I hope
That my dream never haunted you
My heart is telling you
How much I wanted you
Gloomy Sunday
“Gloomy Sunday," Rezso Seress, 1933
When a joke takes shape, the unconscious, whatever its
location, seems to provide access to hidden knowledge. Freud explains how jokes
crystallize in the unconscious: “a joking allusion…emerges without my being
able to follow…preparatory stages in my thoughts.” (p. 168) Indeed, the moment
in which the joke is propelled to the surface, surprising us with its meaning,
is the tiniest of eureka! moments, a brief moment of inspiration. A joke is often
a type of involuntary verbal imagery retrieved by chance.
A joke has quite
outstandingly the characteristic of being a notion that has occurred to us
‘involuntarily’. What happens is…we have an indefinable feeling…which I can
best compare with an absence, a sudden release of intellectual tension, and
then all at once the joke is there–as a rule ready-clothed in words. (Freud,
p. 167)
* * *
What words did the
hypochondriac have engraved on his tombstone?
“I told you so.”
* * *
If you haven’t already smiled at the hypochondriac joke,
thinking about why it’s funny won’t make you smile. Nothing ruins a joke faster
than a logical explanation. Yes, in the case of the hypochondriac, death
provided vindication through the inevitable illness; beneath the humor of the
vindication is the truism that we all die eventually. But talking about a joke
isn’t like telling or hearing it. Telling and 'getting' a joke involve
putting together incongruous ideas. Our enjoyment of jokes depends on alogical
thinking. For this very reason, books about humor, like Freud’s book on jokes,
or Henri Bergson’s book on laughter, are rarely funny.
* * *
The great humorist
Charles Dickens married a rather slow-witted woman. One evening at a party
Dickens told a joke that elicited much hilarity from everyone but his wife. She
was silent at the time, and indeed, Dickens heard little from her the next day.
But that night, working in his study, Dickens heard his wife upstairs, rocked
by sudden uproarious laughter.
* * *
Timing is at the heart of humor. Unlike dreams and moments
of personal inspiration, telling a joke is a shared experience. Clusters of
neurons must spike in the brains of the teller of jokes and his audience so
closely in time that the telling and hearing of the joke feel almost
simultaneous. The story about Dickens may be untrue, but if it’s well told, we
laugh ruefully about the time lag between Dickens’ telling of his joke and his
wife’s getting it – an ominous sign of their incompatibility.
Certain types of brain damage make it difficult for people
to understand jokes. In the 1990s, researcher Mark Beeman of the National
Institutes of Health studied patients who sustained severe damage to the right
hemisphere of the brain. Although they retained a high degree of language
proficiency, Beeman observed that, “Some of these patients couldn’t understand
jokes or sarcasm or metaphor.” (Lehrer, Jonah, “The neuroscience of Bob Dylan’s
genius,” The Guardian, April 6, 2012)
Noting that patients with injuries to the right hemisphere
had difficulties with maps as well as jokes, Beeman inferred that the right
hemisphere of the brain may be involved in finding subtle connections between apparently
unrelated concepts. If jokes are formed in the unconscious – the place where
the ‘involuntary’ onset of humor gains momentum – and if jokes are understood
in the right brain – should we conclude that the unconscious is located in the
right hemisphere of the brain?
Perhaps though, in consideration of the controversies
surrounding cognitive mapping, and in consideration of the complexities of
neuroscience, it makes more sense to limit this inquiry, and simply to ask
whether, in the case of jokes, unconscious processes may be located in the
right brain.
Beeman theorized that while the left hemisphere of the brain deals with
denotative language, the right hemisphere deals with connotation. Metaphors and
other figures of speech are formulated and understood through the right brain
process of seeking connections between unrelated ideas. Is the right brain also
involved in quickly finding the incongruous ideas that result in a joke? While the equivalency: unconscious = right brain may not
hold for all mental phenomena, it appears to be valid for jokes.
In 1993, Beeman conducted an experiment to explore the
problem-solving functions of the right and left hemispheres of the brain. He
and research partner John Kounios found that while the left brain activity of
conscious analysis created a predictable route to problem-solving,
unconventional problem-solving in the right brain often led to insight.
(Lehrer, The Guardian, 2012) The insights galvanized by right brain activity
turned out to occur at a precise location in the brain, and to have a dynamic describable in terms of neurological process.
Beeeman and Kounios discovered the following
correspondences. Insight, the erupting of the answer into
consciousness (the what), is preceded by a sudden burst of activity in the
brain. This brain activity takes the form of a spiking of gamma-wave rhythm (the how) –
the highest electrical frequency in the brain – thirty milliseconds before inspiration
strikes (the when). Gamma-wave rhythm is thought to accompany the binding of a neuron
network poised to enter consciousness (the why). The spike in gamma-waves can
be observed in the anterior superior temporal gyrus (aSTG) on the surface of
the right hemisphere of the brain (the where).
If researchers have yet to determine what happens in the brain when someone tells a joke, it is more than likely that they will one day describe that neurological process and pinpoint the precise location in the brain – perhaps the aSTG – where jokes are made. Drawing on Beeman’s discoveries, author and journalist Jonah Lehrer gives a descriptive account of inspiration as experienced by an artist: inspiration is preceded by frustration, and is initially felt as coming from a source outside the self. (Lehrer, The Guardian, 2012) Lehrer’s behavioral description of inspiration resembles, in certain ways, Freud’s account of joke telling. The artist’s frustration is perhaps an extended version of the indefinable moment of “absence” in Freud’s description that is followed by “a sudden release of intellectual tension”. The artist’s sense that inspiration comes from the Muse may be a grander version of the joke teller’s feeling that the joke has occurred to him ‘involuntarily’.
Freud didn’t know from neurons, but he knew from jokes. His
behavioral account of joke telling and his comprehensive, literally analytical,
understanding of the operation of joke techniques in the unconscious provides a
basis for thinking about the similarity of jokes and psychological events
like inspiration. Does a spike in gamma-wave rhythm precede the emergence of a joke?
If so, how many milliseconds before a joke erupts into consciousness?
Does a joke-induced gamma-wave spike happen in the gyrus, or in some other place in
the brain? It might be challenging to test joke formation
experimentally, but it would be revealing to see how closely experimental
results correlate with Freud’s inferential schema.
My daughter organized our seventy-two hour trip to Yalta, which we reached on the cruise ship Shevchenko (named for a poet, of course). Because we were due back in Odessa shortly, we prioritized our tour site wish list. Tanya’s number one priority was to climb the steps to Swallow’s Nest; after that came Livadiya Palace and Chekhov’s house. My priorities were first, to visit Chekhov’s house and the Chekhov Museum, second, to tour Livadiya Palace and third, to survive the 800 steps to Swallow’s Nest. Neither of us especially wanted to experience the heights of Ai-Petri, a mountain overlooking the Black Sea. But when we arrived late on a Friday afternoon, a trip to the top of the mountain by taxi was the only option for that day.
"Freud died on the eve of World War Two, and hasn't written a word since."
– Robert Young, Voices: Psychoanalysis, 1987
My daughter organized our seventy-two hour trip to Yalta, which we reached on the cruise ship Shevchenko (named for a poet, of course). Because we were due back in Odessa shortly, we prioritized our tour site wish list. Tanya’s number one priority was to climb the steps to Swallow’s Nest; after that came Livadiya Palace and Chekhov’s house. My priorities were first, to visit Chekhov’s house and the Chekhov Museum, second, to tour Livadiya Palace and third, to survive the 800 steps to Swallow’s Nest. Neither of us especially wanted to experience the heights of Ai-Petri, a mountain overlooking the Black Sea. But when we arrived late on a Friday afternoon, a trip to the top of the mountain by taxi was the only option for that day.
And it turned out to be wonderful: the spectacular views,
the horses running free, the Tatar hamlet at the top, and the small restaurant
where they cooked meat on an open fire and served borscht and grass tea.
From the top of Ai-Petri we saw the Crimean coast surrounded by the churning sea. Back down the mountainside at the foot of Ai-Petri stood the Alupka Palace where Winston Churchill was lodged by Stalin during the Yalta
Conference in 1945. There the elder statesman fretted about being away from
the center of things, and very likely got off his game. The
Livadiya Palace, where the conference was held, housed
the apartments of the frail Franklin Delano Roosevelt and other members of the American delegation. When not attending conference sessions, Churchill languished in his coastal villa. There was nothing to be done, of course. Stalin was the host. The joke was on
Churchill, and then on Poland and Eastern Europe. But no joke is big enough for that folly.
No comments:
Post a Comment